TY - CHAP
T1 - Forms of coding in sentence comprehension during reading
AU - Black, Maria
AU - Coltheart, Max
AU - Byng, Sally
N1 - First published 1987, eBook version published 2016 (9781315630427), additional physical format printed 2017
PY - 1987
Y1 - 1987
N2 - It is widely believed that the syntactic processor operating upon sentences during reading needs to use a phonological buffer, upon whose contents this processor operates. We provide evidence for this view using verb-gapped sentences such as Sue polished the table and Frank the shoes. Subjects were asked to detect semantic anomalies in such sentences, the anomalies being dependent upon the relationship of the verb to the object noun phrase in the second clause (as in Sue polished the table and Frank the sea). Anomaly detection was slower or less accurate for those anomalous sentences where a homophone of the verb would yield an acceptable sentence (as in he rights injustices and she books), suggesting that when the syntactic processor is dealing with verb gaps, the filling of the gap involves consulting phonological representations from the first clause. It turned out that processing of these sentences was also impeded when the verb and its gap differed in number, even when past tenses, where singular and plural verbs do not differ in phonology, are used (as in Your friends mended the car and your brother the bike). Implications of this result, and of the homophone effect, for modelling sentence processing are considered.
AB - It is widely believed that the syntactic processor operating upon sentences during reading needs to use a phonological buffer, upon whose contents this processor operates. We provide evidence for this view using verb-gapped sentences such as Sue polished the table and Frank the shoes. Subjects were asked to detect semantic anomalies in such sentences, the anomalies being dependent upon the relationship of the verb to the object noun phrase in the second clause (as in Sue polished the table and Frank the sea). Anomaly detection was slower or less accurate for those anomalous sentences where a homophone of the verb would yield an acceptable sentence (as in he rights injustices and she books), suggesting that when the syntactic processor is dealing with verb gaps, the filling of the gap involves consulting phonological representations from the first clause. It turned out that processing of these sentences was also impeded when the verb and its gap differed in number, even when past tenses, where singular and plural verbs do not differ in phonology, are used (as in Your friends mended the car and your brother the bike). Implications of this result, and of the homophone effect, for modelling sentence processing are considered.
UR - https://doi.org/10.4324/9781315630427
UR - http://www.scopus.com/inward/record.url?scp=84983629772&partnerID=8YFLogxK
M3 - Chapter
SN - 9780863770845
SN - 0863770843
SN - 9780863770838
SN - 9781138641549
T3 - Psychology library editions: Cognitive science
SP - 655
EP - 672
BT - Attention and performance XII
A2 - Coltheart, Max
PB - Lawrence Erlbaum Associates
CY - Hove, UK
T2 - 12th Attention and Performance Meeting
Y2 - 1 July 1986
ER -